- Currents
- Season 1
- Episode 17
Vision Scientist Explains Why These Praying Mantises Are Wearing 3D Glasses
Released on 08/08/2019
[Narrator] Who doesn't love a good old 3D movie?
♪ Let's all go to the lobby ♪
Do you know what I hate
is when producers make me wear silly props
that make me look like an idiot.
[thudding]
But you know who doesn't look stupid in 3D glasses
are these tiny preying mantises.
For the passed six years,
researchers at Newcastle University
have been strapping tiny 3D glasses
onto these little critters to study how their 3D vision
might work and how they might apply
those findings to robots.
I'm Jenny Read, I'm professor of vision science
at the Institute of Neuroscience, Newcastle University, UK.
To learn more we caught up with Jenny Read,
who's leading this research.
Tell us about this system that you are studying?
So I'm studying preying mantises
and in particular how they do 3D or stereoscopic vision.
So you use the different views that your two eyes see
to work out how far away things are.
And that's an ability that was only discovered in humans
in the 19th century, and used to be thought, you know,
really complicated and only higher mammals
like humans and monkeys and maybe cats can do,
but back in the 1980s it was discovered
that actually insects have this ability as well,
or at least one insect, the preying mantis.
So, when I heard about that,
I got really interested and thought,
we need to find out how that works.
How is these tiny insect brains
achieving stereoscopic vision?
Are they doing it in the same way as we do?
Which would be really interesting,
because I think then that would tell us
that there's only one good way of doing stereo vision,
but the other possibility is that maybe insects
have evolved a completely separate way
of doing stereo vision.
In that case, that would also be really fascinating
to find out how they've done it
and maybe get new ideas
for how you can achieve stereoscopic vision in machines.
So this is one of my favorite methodologies
of all time, can you talk about
how you went about studying this system?
How you went about attaching 3D glasses to mantises?
So that was the first puzzle that we had to solve
when we were beginning this study.
So I mentioned the initial research
in the 1980s on insect 3D vision.
That used prisms,
and it's a really beautiful and clever technique
and because prisms bend the light rays,
that has the effect of basically
stereoscopically moving everything either
closer or further away,
but we couldn't use that for the experiments I wanted to do.
Because in order to answer these questions,
we needed to present much more complicated 3D images to it,
so I needed to find some way
of presenting 3D images just like we do for humans,
so we spent a lot of time trying various techniques,
but in the end, the only one that's actually worked for us
is using colored filters.
So you might be familiar with these red blue 3D glasses
that I don't know, you used to get in cereal packets
when I was a kid and in the 3D movies back in the '50s.
That technologies kind of been superseded for humans,
but for the insects it still works really well.
The only this is we didn't want to use red light
because insects, most of them, including mantids,
don't see red light at all.
It's just not a wavelength that they're sensitive to.
So we shifted everything towards shorter wavelengths
and we used blue and green light.
So the idea is we have a computer monitor,
and we display the left image,
say on the blue channel,
and the right image on the green channel
and then with these colored filters,
we ensure that each eye of the insect
sees only the appropriate channel,
and that's work done by my very talented postdoc,
Vivek Nityananda, and he spent so much time
figuring out how to achieve this
and ironing out all the problems
that we encountered along the way.
And of course, as you say, the big problem
was how do you attach glasses to an insect, right?
They don't have any ears that you could put glasses on with,
and so Vivek came up with a harmless form of glue
made out of beeswax and resin,
so natural ingredients that aren't going to harm the animal,
and with a wax melter
he just melts a little daub of beeswax,
places it on the mantis' forehead,
and then with tiny 3D glasses that he's cut out previously,
he holds them with tweezers and just pops them in place.
The beeswax dries within seconds
and then the glasses are held in place
in front of the mantids eyes.
And you might think they would be bothered by that,
but actually they seem really indifferent to it.
So can you elaborate a bit
on what kinds of images you're showing to these mantises
and then what you are learning from
how they're reacting to such images?
So here I'd like to distinguish between
basically three prongs of our research.
We're pursuing behavioral, electro-physiological,
and computational strands of the research.
So the behavior is trying to figure out, okay,
what can their stereo vision do?
How do we break it?
What kind of images does it work with?
What kind of images does it not work with?
And this is something vision scientists do all the time
and it helps us figure out, basically,
the algorithms that the brain is using
in order to achieve 3D vision
and that's where the computational part comes in.
And the thing is for the behavioral experiments,
we're basically relying on the animals
to tell us what they can see.
So, they're experiencing a 3D illusion in our experiments,
if they reach out and catch,
or try and catch the virtual object.
So the trouble is that limits us
because whatever we display
has to be attractive enough to elicit a strike.
If the animals see something in 3D but isn't interested,
it'll just sit there, and then of course we don't know,
is it seeing anything?
Is it not seeing anything?
So, Vivek's first job was to try and figure out
a really attractive stimulus
that would drive the behavior really well,
and what we've come up with was a black disk
that just spirals around the computer screen,
getting, in ever decreasing circles
and it comes to rest just in front of the mantis,
and that really seems to get them going.
So that was our first stimulus
that we used to basically prove
that the glasses were working.
And having established that,
then we could use more complicated images,
so we've been using images
made up of black and white patterns of dots,
and the reason for this is we want to then manipulate things
like the correlation of the pattern between the two eyes.
Basically, human stereo vision works
by cross-correlating the two eyes' images.
So it's where your brain is kind of
sliding across the images and going
Ah, yep, that is the best match
where the correlation is highest.
And so that was my starting point for mantis stereo vision.
So, with these random dot patterns
we manipulated the correlation
between the left and right eye images.
We know in humans that totally messes with your 3D vision,
but to our great surprise,
this just didn't bother the mantids at all.
They were able to carry on striking
and grabbing the virtual prey
even when there was no correlation at all
between the pattern of light and dark
in the two eyes' images.
So you might say, Well, if there wasn't any correlation,
how could you define where the prey was?
And the answer is it's the relation
where things are changing in the image.
So it's a stereo vision
that's based entirely on motion,
or at least on change, and it actually,
as far as we know, doesn't work at all with static images.
So it's really completely different
in that sense from our own human stereo vision.
What were you doing to kind of look inside the brains
to determine what is going on in the neurobiology here
when they are looking at these images?
My other extremely talented post-doc, Ronny Rosner,
has been looking at that
and that's been a real technical challenge
because even though he's extremely experienced
in insect neurophysiology,
people don't tend to work with preying mantises,
so he had to figure out how to get into their brains,
how to recall the activity from individual neurons,
nerve cells inside their brain,
and he had to do this while
they were watching these 3D displays.
Ronny's found neurons in the mantis brain
which attune to positions in 3D space,
so a neuron that might like objects here,
or a neuron that might like objects over here.
And by like I mean it's firing more spikes of voltage.
And that seems to be telling the animal
where things are in the space around it.
So, putting that all together,
how would a system like that
that's going on in the mantis insect brain
differ from how we humans perceive 3D objects?
So we're still trying to work out the details of that
but the kind of preliminary model
I have in my mind at the moment,
which we're trying to write computer programs
to instantiate and test,
is that actually the take home for me
is it's much more similar to humans
at the neural level than I have expected.
We thought originally that mantis stereopsis
might be super simple,
like maybe you have inputs in the left eye
and inputs in the right eye
and they're combined very late
so that you basically have an and gate
just before you decide whether to strike.
And if you see something in the left eye and the right eye,
bingo, you strike.
But actually Ronny's finding many classes of neurons
that attuned to stereoscopic distance at all levels
of the brain and he's found feedback loops.
So that's suggesting quite a complicated circuit,
so I think it's been surprising
how similar the neural basis of 3D vision seems to be
in the insect compared to the mammal.
But, I think the difference is maybe at the front end.
So whereas these computations in humans
are being done on the pattern of light and dark
in the two eyes,
in the mantis they seem to be done on the pattern of change.
So where things are changing.
So I personally believe that this research
can stand perfectly find on its own,
it's great, these are awesome findings,
but there's a bigger picture here, I think,
with applications towards robotics in particular.
Can you elaborate how you might take
what you've learned with this study
and then apply them to machines
that are moving around in the real world?
See, I'm very interested in this
because I tend to think of insects as kind of tiny,
amazingly efficient robots,
and so of course it's hugely interesting
to think how could we take what we learned from insects
and put it into robotics.
I take two main lessons from what we've learned so far
about mantis stereopsis.
I think working with change is a really interesting idea,
and not something that's been used a lot
in the machine stereo-literature so far.
And the fact that it works so successfully for mantids
suggest to me that it might be worth taking seriously
for some robotic applications.
Again, you have to think carefully about the applications,
but maybe if you have a drone
that is moving and wants to come and pick something up,
just like a mantis catching a prey
that is a particular distance,
maybe it would work for that kind of application.
Cool and thank you for taking the time to chat today.
Absolute pleasure, thanks very much.
How the Disco Clam Uses Light to Fight Super-Strong Predators
Architect Explains How Homes Could be 3D Printed on Mars and Earth
Scientist Explains How Rare Genetics Allow Some to Sleep Only 4 Hours a Night
Scientist Explains Unsinkable Metal That Could Prevent Disasters at Sea
Is Invisibility Possible? An Inventor and a Physicist Explain
Scientist Explains Why Her Lab Taught Rats to Drive Tiny Cars
Mycologist Explains How a Slime Mold Can Solve Mazes
How the Two-Hour Marathon Limit Was Broken
Research Suggests Cats Like Their Owners as Much as Dogs
Researcher Explains Deepfake Videos
Scientist Explains How to Study the Metabolism of Ultra High Flying Geese
Hurricane Hunter Explains How They Track and Predict Hurricanes
Scientist Explains Viral Fish Cannon Video
A Biohacker Explains Why He Turned His Leg Into a Hotspot
Scientist Explains What Water Pooling in Kilauea's Volcanic Crater Means
Bill Nye Explains the Science Behind Solar Sailing
Vision Scientist Explains Why These Praying Mantises Are Wearing 3D Glasses
Why Some Cities Are Banning Facial Recognition Technology
Scientist's Map Explains Climate Change
Scientist Explains How Moon Mining Would Work
Scientist Explains How She Captured Rare Footage of a Giant Squid
Doctor Explains How Sunscreen Affects Your Body
Stranger Things is Getting a New Mall! But Today Malls Are Dying. What Happened?
The Limits of Human Endurance Might Be Our Guts
Meet the First College Students to Launch a Rocket Into Space
Scientist Explains Why Dogs Can Smell Better Than Robots
A Harvard Professor Explains What the Avengers Can Teach Us About Philosophy
NASA Twin Study: How Space Changes Our Bodies
What the Black Hole Picture Means for Researchers
Scientist Explains How to Levitate Objects With Sound
Why Scientists and Artists Want The Blackest Substances on Earth
Biologist Explains How Drones Catching Whale "Snot" Helps Research
Researcher Explains Why Humans Can't Spot Real-Life Deepfake Masks
Doctor Explains What You Need to Know About The Coronavirus
VFX Artist Breaks Down This Year's Best Visual Effects Nominees
How Doctors on Earth Treated a Blood Clot in Space
Scientist Explains Why Some Cats Eat Human Corpses
Voting Expert Explains How Voting Technology Will Impact the 2020 Election
Doctor Explains What You Need to Know About Pandemics
ER Doctor Explains How They're Handling Covid-19
Why This Taste Map Is Wrong
Q&A: What's Next for the Coronavirus Pandemic?
Why Captive Tigers Can’t Be Reintroduced to the Wild
How Covid-19 Immunity Compares to Other Diseases
5 Mistakes to Avoid as We Try to Stop Covid-19
How This Emergency Ventilator Could Keep Covid-19 Patients Alive
Why NASA Made a Helicopter for Mars
Theoretical Physicist Breaks Down the Marvel Multiverse
Former NASA Astronaut Explains Jeff Bezos's Space Flight
Physics Student Breaks Down Gymnastics Physics
What Do Cities Look Like Under a Microscope?
Inside the Largest Bitcoin Mine in The U.S.
How Caffeine Has Fueled History
How Mushroom Time-Lapses Are Filmed
Why You’ll Fail the Milk Crate Challenge
Why Vegan Cheese Doesn't Melt
How 250 Cameras Filmed Neill Blomkamp's Demonic
How Meme Detectives Stop NFT Fraud
How Disney Designed a Robotic Spider-Man
How Online Conspiracy Groups Compare to Cults
Dune Costume Designers Break Down Dune’s Stillsuits
Korean Phrases You Missed in 'Squid Game'
Why Scientists Are Stress Testing Tardigrades
Every Prototype that Led to a Realistic Prosthetic Arm
Why the Toilet Needs an Upgrade
How Animals Are Evolving Because of Climate Change
How Stop-Motion Movies Are Animated at Aardman
Astronomer Explains How NASA Detects Asteroids
Are We Living In A Simulation?
Inside the Journey of a Shipping Container (And Why the Supply Chain Is So Backed Up)
The Science of Slow Aging